The Substance of False Confessions

Brandon Garrett -University of Virginia School of Law

Posted in , , ,

The scholarship of interrogations has taken a turn from procedure to substance. The Supreme Court’s landmark criminal procedure rulings regulating modern psychological interrogations remain static, inviting lingering decades-long debates over whether the Court correctly decided decisions such as Miranda. Meanwhile, psychologists increasingly study not the legal regulation of interrogations, but police practices inside the interrogation room. Saul Kassin, Gisli Gudjonsson, Richard Ofshe, Richard Leo, and many others have shed light, through landmark experimental and empirical work, on how modern psychological interrogation techniques can generate false confessions. The American Psychology and Law Association released a White Paper on the subject, surveying the literature and recommending a series of changes to interrogation procedures.1

One important motivation for this change in scholarly focus is that the problem of false confessions has become far from academic. In the past, it was often difficult to prove a confession false, since the confession itself provided such compelling evidence of guilt. Interrogation room errors disguised themselves. The development of DNA technology upended the interrogation landscape. DNA testing made it possible in certain cases involving a stranger rape to obtain highly probative genetic information concerning the identity of the rapist. Postconviction DNA testing has now exonerated 252 convicts, 41 of whom falsely confessed to rapes and murders. As a result, there is a new awareness among scholars, legislators, courts, prosecutors, police departments, and the public that innocent people falsely confess, often due to psychological pressure placed upon them during police interrogations. States have enacted laws requiring videotaping or recording of the interrogation process, at least for certain types of crimes, and state supreme courts have required or encouraged electronic recording of interrogations. Police departments in far greater numbers have voluntarily adopted recording protocols for interrogations.

My article, “The Substance of False Confessions,” examines the substance of the unusual set of false confessions rendered by persons later exonerated by postconviction DNA testing. I obtained their original criminal trial transcripts and most of their confession statements.2 Doing so shed light on the phenomenon of confession contamination. Police may, intentionally or unintentionally, prompt the suspect on how the crime happened. Yet false confessions do not happen simply by happenstance. They are carefully constructed during an interrogation and then reconstructed during any criminal trial that follows. Constitutional criminal procedure does not regulate this critical phase of an interrogation. The Constitution requires that the ubiquitous Miranda warnings be provided and that the bare admission of guilt have been made voluntarily under the totality of the circumstances. That admission of guilt, while important, is only a part of the interrogation process. After the bare admission of guilt, police conduct a lengthy “confession-making” phase. Much of the power of a confession derives from the lengthy narrative that follows. For a person to confess in a convincing way, he must be able to say much more than “I did it.” The confession narrative not only plays an important role in fleshing out the suspect’s motive and culpability, but properly used, it can be used to test the accuracy of the confession. Police are trained to evaluate the suspect’s knowledge of how the crime occurred, by asking open-ended questions to assess whether the suspect can freely volunteer specific details that only the true culprit could know.

The false confessions in DNA cases all involved very long interrogations, and most involved vulnerable juveniles or mentally disabled individuals. In all cases but one (95% or 36 of the 38 exonerees for whom trial or pre-trial records could be obtained) police reported that suspects confessed to a series of specific details concerning how the crime occurred. As the prosecutor in Robert Miller’s case briefly put it, “He supplied detail after detail after detail after detail. And details that only but the killer could have known.”3 The non-public facts contained in confession statements then became the centerpiece of the State’s case. Although defense counsel moved to exclude almost all of these confessions from the trial, courts found each to be voluntary and admissible, often citing to the apparent reliability of the confessions. The facts were typically the focus of the State’s closing arguments to the jury. Even after DNA testing excluded these people, courts sometimes initially denied relief, citing to the seeming reliability of these confessions. For example, Nathaniel Hatchett had been excluded by DNA testing at the time of trial, but postconviction, the court concluded that “the prosecution presented overwhelming evidence” where the “defendant’s statement included information that only the perpetrator of the crimes would know.”4 The ironic result is that the public learned about these false confessions in part because of the contaminated facts. These false confessions were so persuasive, detailed, and believable that they resulted in convictions upheld during appeals and habeas review. After years passed, and appeals and habeas petitions were all denied and dismissed, these convicts had no option left but to seek the DNA testing that finally proved their confessions false.

False confessions uncovered by DNA testing are almost certainly not representative of other false confessions, much less confessions more generally. The 40 cases examined, consisting chiefly of confessions to 1980’s rape-murders, cannot speak to how often people confess falsely. While unusual, only in such examples of known false confessions can one assess whether detailed or supposedly non-public facts contaminated a confession. These data provide a set of examples of a very troubling problem that deserves further study.

One reason why these examples are important is that courts, lawmakers, and executives, while perhaps increasingly aware that false confessions can occur, continue to ignore that there is even a possibility that false confessions may appear uncannily reliable precisely because of the insidious problem of confession contamination.  Justice Alito, concurring in U.S. Supreme Court in District Attorney’s Association v. Osborne, noted that “[a]fter conviction, in an unsuccessful attempt to obtain parole, respondent confessed in detail to the crime.”5 Of course, if it is true that Osborne is innocent, he certainly could have known the details of his case. His “confession” to the parole board was by definition contaminated because he could very easily admit guilt “in detail” based on the victim’s account of the crime that he heard at his own trial. In one additional recent example, Virginia’s Governor granted a conditional but not full pardon in the “Norfolk Four” case, despite DNA exclusions, citing to the “cumulative power” of confession statements, but never acknowledging that the statements could have been contaminated.6 Those examples share the view that a detailed, accurate confession must be true. However, a contaminated confession statement can be detailed, seemingly accurate, and also false.

Nor should scrutiny of the confession-making process be limited to the problem of false confessions. The confession-making process can generate accurate convictions, but by using corrosive and socially intolerable means. Anne Coughlin has written an important article examining the victim-blaming narratives endorsed by leading training manuals and employed to minimize the acts of a suspect during interrogations. While social scientists have examined how the use of psychological techniques may coerce and produce false confessions, they have not examined the other distortions such strategies produce, nor their effect on the legitimacy of criminal investigations and adjudication. As Coughlin develops, “[v]ictim-blaming is incompatible with the contemporary goals of rape law, and the police should stop feeding those stock stories to accused rapists.”7 The problem of contamination is thus not limited to the accuracy of the crime narrative, but also extends to the interrogation room shaping of mens rea and of defenses that the suspect might otherwise rely upon. We should look more carefully at the substance of scripts used by police interrogators and the substance of statements by suspects regarding defenses and mens rea, and not just crime details.

Perhaps the prolonged involvement of law enforcement in generating a confession, true or false, should itself raise concerns about possible contamination. Barry Feld’s work reviewing recorded juvenile interrogations in Minnesota suggests that the typical interrogation of a suspect lasts much less than an hour, and not hours, and without prolonged questioning, elaborate ruses, or schemes designed to produce coercion. Rather, in those interrogations of juveniles, many of which produced confession statements, police used open-ended questioning designed to elicit a narrative, together with questions designed to directly confront them with an accusation that they committed the crime or accusing them of lying. Other studies suggest that the vast majority of interrogations, even in more serious cases, are fairly short and benign. Feld asks us to consider, “Are confronting suspects with false evidence and lying during interrogation necessary evils?”8 The over-the-top and elaborate psychological techniques used in lengthy interrogations described in police manuals and used in many of these DNA exonerees’ harrowing interrogations may not be the norm. Perhaps such lengthy interrogations are needed in certain difficult and serious cases. However, scholars are right to increasingly scrutinize lengthy interrogations using complex confession-making tactics—particularly if the ornate storytelling, narrative formation, fabrication, deception, threats, psychological manipulation, and the like occurs in darkness, absent any electronic recording.

A series of reforms could reorient our criminal system towards the substance of confessions. First, although constitutional criminal procedure could regulate reliability, such constitutional change is unlikely. However, an understanding of the vulnerability of confessions to contamination can also inform courts reviewing trials postconviction, particularly in cases involving persons vulnerable to suggestion, such as juveniles and mentally disabled individuals. Second, unless interrogations are recorded in their entirety, courts may not be aware of what transpired during the confession-making process, much less detect contamination of facts, especially when no DNA testing can be performed. In recent years there has been a concerted movement towards reform. In response to some of these false confessions, state legislatures, police departments, and courts have increasingly required videotaping of entire interrogations. Third, additional police procedures can safeguard reliability, such as procedures intended to assure against contamination, to assess suggestibility of vulnerable suspects, and to avoid coercion postadmission.

Contamination of confessions is to a great degree preventable, and the remedy involves making interrogations less secretive. The remedy—electronic recording combined with reliability review—may produce benefits to law enforcement and to society beyond detecting errors. The Supreme Court complained in Miranda, “Interrogation still takes place in privacy. Privacy results in secrecy and this in turn results in a gap in our knowledge as to what in fact goes on in the interrogation rooms.”9 Recording will help to close that gap in our knowledge about interrogation practices. In most cases, police will document professional, brief, accurate, and unprompted confessions. However, recording and review will also help to identify the hopefully dwindling number of contaminated false confessions, which before the advent of DNA technology had remained shrouded in secrecy and darkness. Sunlight in the interrogation room will bring interrogation practices out into the open, so that we can better understand and evaluate them. That shift towards reviewing the substance of confessions has the potential to enhance confidence in both the accuracy and legitimacy of the criminal process.

Acknowledgments:

Copyright © 2010 Stanford Law Review.

Brandon L. Garrett is an Associate Professor of Law at the University of Virginia School of Law.

This Legal Workshop Editorial is based on the following Law Review Article: Brandon L. Garrett, The Substance of False Confessions, 62 STAN. L. REV. 1051 (2010).

  1. See Saul M. Kassin et al., Police-Induced Confessions: Risk Factors and Recommendations, 34 LAW & HUM. BEHAV. (forthcoming 2010), available at http://www.springerlink.com/content/85vh322j085784t0/fulltext.pdf.
  2. Much of this material has been scanned and posted online as a research resource, available at: http://www.law.virginia.edu/html/librarysite/garrett_falseconfess.htm. The body of false confessions that I studied promises to continue to grow.  The study set closed in February, 2010 with the 250th DNA exoneration, and as a result, the Article notes that the 251st and 252nd DNA exonerations, which occurred as the Article approached publication, were not included.  Both DNA exonerations, however, involved false confessions.  That of Ted Bradford in Washington state involved a false confession reported to have included “details that would only be known to the rapist.”  Mark Morey, Jurors Find Bradford Innocent of Rape, YAKIMA HERALD-REPUBLIC, Feb. 11, 2010.  That of Anthony Caravella in Florida, involved a confession that initially included details inconsistent with the crime, but which over a series of interrogations was reported to include accurate details, including information “suggested to him by leading questions.” Paula McMahon, DNA Result Just One Troubling Aspect in Convicted Man’s Case, SOUTH FLA. SUN SENTINEL, Sept. 4, 2009.  After the publication of the Article, the 253d DNA exoneration occurred, and it involved yet another false confession that was reported to have included inside information concerning the crime.  Rachel Barnhart, Innocence Project: Frank Sterling Cleared of Manville Murder, Christie Confesses, WHAM.com, March 28, 2010.
  3. Trial Transcript at 1292, State v. Miller, CRF-87-963 (Okla. Dist. Ct. May 19, 1988).
  4. People v. Hatchett, No. 211131, 2000 WL 33419396, at *1 (Mich. Ct. App. May 19, 2000).
  5. 129 S.Ct. 2308, 2324 (2009) (Alito, J., concurring).
  6. Mike McPhate, Kaine’s Full Statement on “Norfolk Four” Case, WASH. POST, Aug. 6, 2009, available at http://voices.washingtonpost.com/virginiapolitics/2009/08/kaines_full_statement_on_norfo.html?sid=ST2009080602217.
  7. Anne M. Coughlin, Interrogation Stories, 95 Va. L. Rev. 1660 (2009).
  8. Barry C. Feld, Police Interrogation of Juveniles: An Empirical Study of Policy and Practice, 97 J. CRIM. L. & CRIMINOLOGY 219, 316 (2006).
  9. Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436, 448 (1966).

Post a Comment (all fields are required)

You must be logged in to post a comment.


moncler coats ugg kensington ugg italia ugg kengät the north face portugal canada goose online ugg boots canada cheap ugg boots uk replica uggs new uggs 2013 imitation ugg cheapest ugg boots goyard handbags barneys goyard wallet men goyard handbags barneys goyard prices where can i buy insanity workout insanity workout for cheap discount insanity workout goyard tote singapore borsa celine air max portugal nike air max 90 nike shox rivalry pas cher air max bw femme air max enfant pas cher fendi sunglasses 2014 buy fendi online nike free run 2 pas cher παπουτσια nike air max baratas nike air max 90 tilbud fendi store nike blazer pas cher nike id france fendi outlet online nike free tilbud coach christmas sale gucci christmas sale louis vuitton xmas lv christmas sale michael kors christmas sets christmas nike shoes prada christmas christmas ugg boots uggs christmas ornament uggs for christmas discounted uggs botas ugg no brasil botas ugg baratas online celine borse air max schweiz nike shox femme nike schoenen nike air max bw pas cher fendi 2014 fendi shop fendi outlet online nike greece zapatillas nike baratas zapatillas nike air max fendi online store nike blazer femme nike store portugal fendi handbags outlet nike danmark buy michael kors michael kors factory outlet michael kors watches nordstrom canada goose jacket outlet canadian goose outlet buy michael kors michael kors factory outlet michael kors watches nordstrom buy michael kors michael kors factory outlet michael kors watches nordstrom canada goose jacket outlet canadian goose outlet canadian goose outlet moncler outlet moncler outlet lv belts men louis vuitton belts for men nike shox nz louis vuitton singapore website cheap nike free runs nike free on sale discount nike shox cheap nike shox